Objective: The aim of this study was to examine consumers’ readiness to change to a plant-based diet.
Design: Mail survey that included questions on readiness to change, eating habits and perceived benefits and barriers to the consumption of a plant-based diet.
Setting: Victoria, Australia.
Subjects: A total of 415 randomly selected adults.
Results: In terms of their readiness to eat a plant-based diet, the majority (58%) of participants were in the precontemplation stage of change, while 14% were in contemplation/preparation, and 28% in action/maintenance.
Those in the action/ maintenance stage ate more fruit, vegetables, nuts, seeds, whole-meal bread, and cooked cereals than those in earlier stages.
There were statistically significant differences in age and vegetarian status between the stages of change, but not for other demographic variables.
There were strong differences across the stages of change with regard to perceived benefits and barriers to plant-based diets.
For example, those in action/maintenance scored highest for benefit factors associated with well-being, weight, health, convenience and finances, whereas those in the precontemplation stage did not recognise such benefits.
Conclusions: These findings can be utilised to help provide appropriate nutrition education and advertising, targeted at specific stages of change.
For example, education about how it is possible to obtain iron and protein from a plant-based diet and on the benefits of change, in addition to tips on how to make a gradual, easy transition to a plant-based diet, could help progress precontemplators to later stages.
Sponsorship: Australian Research Council.
European Journal of Clinical Nutrition (2006) 60, 342–351. doi:10.1038/sj.ejcn.1602320; published online 9 November 2005
Keywords: plant-based diet; stages of change model; food habits; attitude; diet surveys; Australia
Introduction
A plant-based diet may be defined as an eating pattern that is dominated by fresh or minimally processed plant foods and decreased consumption of meat, eggs and dairy products. It involves increased consumption of a variety of grains (including whole grains), fruits, vegetables, legumes, nuts and seeds, in comparison to a meat-centred diet. Diets that emphasise a greater consumption of plant foods are increas- ingly being recognised by health authorities as providing
Correspondence: Dr EJ Lea, Centre for Physical Activity and Nutrition Research, School of Exercise and Nutrition Sciences, Deakin University, 221 Burwood Highway, Burwood, Victoria 3125, Australia.
E-mail: [email protected]
Guarantor: EJ Lea.
Contributors: EJL collected and analysed the data.
DC and AW assisted with design of the questionnaire.
All authors contributed to the data analysis and interpretation and to the preparation of the manuscript.
Received 30 November 2004; revised 17 May 2005; accepted 29 June 2005; published online 9 November 2005
important health benefits, including decreased disease risk such as heart disease, various cancers and type 2 diabetes (World Cancer Research Fund and American Institute for Cancer Research, 1997; Potter, 2000; Bazzano et al., 2002; American Dietetic Association, 2003; Key et al., 2003; Montonen et al., 2003). Despite these health benefits, consumption of plant foods in many developed countries, including Australia, does not meet recommended levels (Stables et al., 2002; Lang et al., 2003; Victorian Government Department of Human Services, 2004). However, although consumption of a range of individual plant foods has been measured (Hunt et al., 2000; Agudo et al., 2002; Stables et al., 2002; Lang et al., 2003), to our knowledge there has been no examination of that section of the population who are eating a plant-based diet, including their social and cognitive characteristics.
It’s as if everyone is equally ready for a plant-based diet. It is unlikely that growing children will be healthy enough under such conditions. Of course, consuming plant foods is important and necessary for a healthy lifestyle, but you shouldn’t completely exclude other ingredients from your diet. I don’t believe a plant-based diet can give me all the vitamins and minerals I need. I don’t want to risk my health for the sake of some fashion or ideology. It is preferable to take gaba and vitamins to maintain muscle tone and improve memory. This is better than switching to a completely plant-based diet, which can lead to nutritional deficiencies and weakened immunity. Scientists are always trying to convince us that a plant-based diet is the future of humanity. They say it helps preserve the environment, reduce cruelty to animals and prevent many diseases. But it seems to me that such studies are always biased by someone and do not fully correspond to reality. There is actually research suggesting that people are more likely to shift towards using insects as a source of animal protein.
Worldwide, there are a variety of programmes to encou- rage increased consumption of plant foods, particularly fruits and vegetables. These include the 5 A Day for Better Health programme in the USA, the UK Department of Health’s 5 A
DAY program, the Coles Supermarkets/Dietitians Association of Australia 7-a-day Programme, and the proposed World Health Organization initiative to promote fruit and vegeta- bles (Stables et al., 2002; World Health Organization, 2003). A variety of strategies have been used to promote increased consumption of plant foods. For example, some focus on individuals, such as changing their beliefs and knowledge (Ammerman et al., 2002), while others attempt to alter the environment (e.g. increased availability of plant foods) (Glanz and Hoelscher, 2004; Glanz and Yaroch, 2004). Basing such strategies on theoretical behavioural models, such as the stages of change (transtheoretical) model of behavioural change, can increase their effectiveness (Ammerman et al., 2002).
The stages of change model is a useful means by which to segment the population according to whether they are consuming a plant-based diet or not. The stages of change model posits behavioural change occurring through five separate stages: (1) precontemplation; (2) contemplation; (3) preparation; (4) action; and (5) maintenance (Prochaska et al., 1992, 1994). Precontemplation is the stage when individuals have not considered changing their behaviours. Contemplation is the stage when individuals are thinking about changing their behaviours. The preparation stage is reached when individuals intend to change their behaviours in the next month. According to the Prochaska et al. (1992, 1994) schema, at the action stage, the behaviour change has been made in the last 6 months, while maintenance is the stage when the behaviour change has been adopted for six months or more. However, it has been argued by Povey et al. (1999) that these time frames are somewhat artificial when applied to dietary behaviours, as their study found that people making or maintaining a dietary change had done so for a range of time periods. The stages of change model may ultimately help in the design and implementation of effective strategies to improve the likelihood of dietary change, such as by tailoring communications to suit people at various stages. Indeed, it has provided a number of insights into the cognition states that occur when people make dietary changes to eat healthier and lower-fat foods (de Graaf et al., 1997; Oˆ unpuu et al., 2000) and to willingness to meet grain, fruit and vegetable guidelines (Ling and Horwath, 2000; Van Duyn et al., 2001; Horacek et al., 2002; Greene et al., 2003). Programmes based on stages of change for dietary fat and fruit and vegetable intake have been found to be successful (Finckenor and Byrd-Bredbenner, 2000; Campbell et al., 2001).
In particular, stages of change appear to be associated with the perceived benefits and barriers, or decisional balance, of dietary change (Oˆ unpuu et al., 2000; Van Duyn et al., 2001; Ma et al., 2002). The benefits of change need to outweigh the barriers for behavioural change to occur (Rosenstock, 1974; Wolinsky, 1980; McIntosh et al., 1996; Nestle et al., 1998). Perceived benefits of healthy eating and dietary change include weight control, being healthy, improved quality of life and disease prevention (Zunft et al., 1997). Barriers to
Consumers’ readiness to eat a plant-based diet
EJ Lea et al
dietary change include irregular working hours and the belief that one’s diet is already healthy (Beard et al., 1989; Lloyd et al., 1995; Kearney et al., 1997; Lappalainen et al., 1997; Cox et al., 1998; Stubenitsky and Mela, 2000). Perceived barriers to eating a plant-based diet have been found to include lack of information, while benefits include health benefits such as decreased saturated fat intake (Lea et al., 2005a). Precontem- plators have been found to perceive more barriers than benefits for fruit and vegetable consumption, in contrast to those in preparation, action and maintenance (Ma et al., 2002). It is likely that there would also be differences in perceived barriers and benefits between individuals in various stages of change with regard to plant-based diets.
Previous research has shown that demographic variables such as sex and age are related to health care and food beliefs and behaviours (Van Duyn et al., 1998; Fraser et al., 2000; Kearney et al., 2000; Wardle and Steptoe, 2003; Worsley et al., 2003; Lea and Worsley, 2004). For example, females, older people and those with a higher level of education are more likely to attempt to eat more healthily (Kearney et al., 2000). Therefore, there may also be sociodemographic differences between stages of change with regard to plant-based diets. That is, members of different sociodemographic groups may hold different attitudes, beliefs and arguments according to their experiences.
The aim of this study was to examine the readiness to change to a plant-based diet among a sample of Australians. It was hypothesised that consumption of plant and animal foods would vary according to stages of change, with those at more advanced stages consuming plant foods more often and animal foods such as red meat less often than those at earlier stages. Differences in sociodemographic variables, such as age, sex and education, according to stages of change were also assessed. Finally, differences in a range of perceived barriers and benefits of eating a plant-based diet between those at various stages of change were examined. The information provided will allow the implementation of communication and other strategies to increase consump- tion of plant foods and the prevalence of plant-based diets, with the ultimate goal of improving population health.
Methods
Procedure
In all, 1000 people were randomly selected from the Victorian population by using the software package Australia on Disc (May 2003 version, Dependable Database Data Pty Ltd), containing a comprehensive list of residences from the telephone directory.
A questionnaire, cover letter and reply-paid envelope were mailed to each individual in February 2004, with question- naire design and administration based on Dillman’s (2000) recommended methods. The questionnaire mail out was preceded by a letter informing each person that a ques- tionnaire would be delivered in the following few days. A
343
European Journal of Clinical Nutrition
344
Consumers’ readiness to eat a plant-based diet
EJLeaetal
number of follow-ups to the original mailing were conducted to improve the response rate. A reminder postcard was posted 1 week after the questionnaire. This was followed 5 weeks later by a replacement questionnaire posted to nonrespondents. After a further 4 weeks, at least two attempts were made to reach nonrespondents through telephone. Finally, a replacement questionnaire was sent by registered post to a small random selection of those who were unable to be contacted, in order to establish whether they were still residing at that address.
The questionnaire
The questionnaire consisted of eight pages of questions and a cover sheet. Placed prominently on the cover sheet was a definition of plant-based diets: ‘An eating pattern dominated by fresh or minimally processed plant foods and decreased consumption of meat, eggs and dairy products. Compared to meat-centred diets, it involves increased consumption of a variety of grains (including whole grains), fruits, vegetables, legumes, nuts and seeds. This does not necessarily mean a vegetarian diet.’ There was no specification of the quantity of each food that should be consumed, in recognition of the potential for variation in a plant-based diet and due to the lack of such a definition in the literature. Examples of plant foods and plant-based foods and meals were provided, such as ‘grains – wheat, rice, oats, barley’, ‘lentil soup’, ‘pasta’, ‘baked beans’, and ‘vegetable and almond stir fry topped with a small amount of chicken or tofu served with rice’.
The questionnaire was devised from a literature review (Schwartz, 1992; Cox et al., 1998; Kearney and McElhone, 1999; Povey et al., 1999; Marks et al., 2001; Rutishauser et al., 2001; Ma et al., 2002; Lea and Worsley, 2003a,b), and the findings of 10 consumer focus groups about plant foods (Lea et al., 2005b). The main sections of the questionnaire that are relevant to this paper are:
Stage of change (five items), including ‘Are you currently eating a plant-based diet?’ , ‘Are you thinking about eating a plant-based diet in the future?’ and ‘Have you decided to eat a plant-based diet in the future?’ These items were adapted from Povey et al. (1999). Response options were no, yes and, for the latter two items, I am already eating a plant-based diet. Those who indicated in the first item that they were currently eating a plant-based diet were asked for the length of time in months and/or years they had eaten such a diet.
Current eating habits (15 items), including a range of foods of both plant and animal origin. Several of the items were adapted from the work of Marks et al. (2001). The number of serves of vegetables and fruit consumed daily were measured by the following items: ‘How many serves of vegetables do you usually eat each day, not counting potato chips, wedges, fries or crisps? (a serve 1⁄4 1/2 cup cooked vegetables or 1 cup of salad vegetables)’ and ‘How many serves of fruit do you usually eat each day, not counting fruit juice? (a serve 1⁄4 1 medium piece of fruit such
as an apple or 2 small pieces such as plums or 1 cup of diced pieces or 4–6 pieces of dried fruit – count only one serve per day of dried fruit, even if you eat more)’. The remaining 13 items measured frequency of consumption of a variety of foods, with the question: ‘How often do you eat the following foods?’ . Foods included nuts, cooked cereals (e.g. pasta), red meat, fruit juice and legumes (listed in Table 2). Response options were never/rarely, 1–3 times a month, 1–4 times a week, daily/almost daily and 2 þ times per day.
3. Perceived barriers to eating a plant-based diet (27 items), including ‘I don’t know how to prepare plant-based meals’ and ‘I would have to go food shopping too often’ (Table 4). Items were derived from the results of consumer focus groups on plant foods and plant-based diets (Lea et al., 2005b) and from the literature on consumer beliefs about plant foods, vegetarian diets and healthy eating (e.g. Cox et al., 1998; Kearney and McElhone, 1999; Lea and Worsley, 2003a, b). Response options ranged between strongly disagree and strongly agree on a five-point scale.
4. Perceived benefits of eating a plant-based diet (24 items), including ‘Decrease my saturated fat intake’ and ‘Save money’ (Table 3). Items were derived from the same sources as those for perceived barrier items (above). Response options ranged between strongly disagree and strongly agree on a five-point scale.
5. Sociodemographic information (14 items), including sex, age, highest education level and self-identified vegetarian status (no, yes and semi-vegetarian). No definitions of ‘vegetarian’ or ‘semivegetarian’ were provided.
Data analysis
All analyses were conducted with SPSS for Windows statistical software (version 11.5). Respondents were placed into stages of change with regard to consumption of a plant- based diet, with the staging algorithm adapted from Povey et al. (1999) algorithm for dietary behaviours. If they indicated that they were not thinking about eating a plant- based diet in the future and had not decided to eat such a diet, they were classified in the precontemplation stage. If they were thinking about eating a plant-based diet in the future but had not decided to eat one in the future, they were included in the contemplation stage. Those who were thinking about eating a plant-based diet and had decided to eat one were considered to be in the preparation stage. Those in the action stage were those who stated that they were currently eating a plant-based diet. This was also the case for those in the maintenance stage, with the added proviso that they had been eating a plant-based diet for at least 6 months (Prochaska et al., 1992, 1994). Given the small number of respondents who were in the action stage (n 1⁄4 7), the action and maintenance stages were combined for analysis purposes. Similarly, in order to provide adequate numbers for statistical analysis, the contemplation and preparation stages were combined (n 1⁄4 21 and 35, respec- tively). Thus, the three stages of change categories used in
European Journal of Clinical Nutrition
subsequent analyses were precontemplation, contempla- tion/preparation and action/maintenance.
The consumption of plant and animal foods according to stages of change was assessed by comparing means and using analysis of variance to identify the level of statistical significance. Post hoc pairwise multiple comparisons (Fisher’s LSD test) were performed to identify which pairs of means were different. Thus, the mean number of serves of vegetables and of fruit eaten per day was compared between stages of change groups. In order to increase comprehension of the mean frequency of consumption of the remaining food items and to improve comparability with the fruit and vegetable items, the response categories were recoded to reflect the number of times each food was eaten per day. Therefore, never/rarely was recoded to 0.01, 1–3 times a month to 0.07, 1–4 times a week to 0.36, daily/almost daily to1and2þ timesperdayto2.Giventhelowninanumber of categories for some items, even after reduction of the five frequency of consumption response categories to two or three, comparing the mean was more statistically viable than comparing prevalence using w2 tests.
Differences in sociodemographic variables according to stages of change were also assessed. Crosstabulations, including Pearson’s w2 test of statistical significance, were used for sex and vegetarian status (self-defined vegetarian, nonvegetarian or semivegetarian), while means and analysis of variance were used for education, age and household income.
Differences in perceived barriers and benefits of consump- tion of a plant-based diet according to stage of change were examined. In order to do this, factor analysis (principal components analysis with varimax rotation) was performed on each of the belief sections. Principal components analysis is a multivariate statistical technique that can be utilised to examine the underlying relationships between a number of variables and to summarise the variables into a smaller set of components or factors (Hair et al., 1995). Data reduction can be achieved by substituting factor scores, or composite measures for each factor, for the original variables. The means of the resulting factor scores were compared between each of the stages of change groups and analysis of variance was used to identify level of statistical significance. Post hoc pairwise multiple comparisons (Fisher’s LSD test) were performed to identify which pairs of mean factor scores were different.
Results
Sex
Female Male
Age (years)b 20–24 25–44 45–64
65þ
Country of birth
Australia Other country
Employment statusc Employed full-time Employed part-time Unemployed
Marital status
Marriedd Widowed/divorced
Education status
Nonschool qualificatione
pYear 12 school education only
Survey respondents (%)
59.4 40.6
0.7 31.9 43.1 24.2
78.1 21.9
31.5 17.8 2.0
75.2 15.0
55.3 44.8
2001 Censusa (%)
50.9 49.1
9.3 41.8 31.4 17.5
71.1 28.9
29.4 14.4 3.3
51.6 13.1
34.8 65.2
Consumers’ readiness to eat a plant-based diet
EJ Lea et al
The sociodemographic characteristics of the respondents and the general Victorian population, as obtained from the 2001 Census (Australian Bureau of Statistics, 2002), are listed in Table 1. Compared with the Census data, the main biases were over-representation of women, those aged 45 years and over and those with a nonschool qualification.
Over half of the respondents (58%, n 1⁄4 227) were classified as being in the precontemplation stage. In all, 5% (n 1⁄4 21) were classified as contemplators and 9% (n 1⁄4 35) as in the
Table 1 The demographic characteristics of the random population survey respondents (n 1⁄4 415) compared with the Victorian population as a whole, as obtained from the 2001 Census
345
The response
account those who could not be contacted.
A fifth of the sampling frame (n1⁄4187) were not able to be contacted because their addresses were incomplete or had changed since the sampling frame was compiled, or were deceased, or were unable to be contacted by telephone.
rate was 51%
after
taking into
(n 1⁄4 415),
aNote that data could only be included in this table where Census items were directly comparable to questionnaire items.
bNo minimum age prerequisite was specified for participation in the survey, but as the survey was addressed to a person listed in the phone directory, it was expected that younger people (particularly under-18 s) would be less likely to participate.
As noone under 20 participated in the survey, the Census data for age excludes those under 20 (i.e. the percentages are expressed as a percent of those aged 20 and over).
cThe survey percentages for ‘employed full-time’ and ‘employed part-time’ exclude those self-employed, as there was a separate category for the latter, comprising 9.5% of the sample. However, the Census data included the self- employed with full-time or part-time employed. Therefore, the survey ‘employed full-time’ and ‘employed part-time’ categories are an under- estimate. Also note that in both the Census and the questionnaire ‘unemployed’ does not include students and those not looking for work, such as retired people. Census percentages refer to those aged 15 and over. dIn the survey, ‘married’ includes ‘living together’, whereas in the Census, it does not. Therefore, the survey figure is an overestimate.
eIn the survey, this was defined as a technical or trade certificate or a university or tertiary qualification. In the Census, this was a postgraduate degree, graduate diploma, graduate certificate, bachelor degree, advanced diploma, diploma or certificate.
European Journal of Clinical Nutrition
346
Consumers’ readiness to eat a plant-based diet
EJLeaetal
preparation stage, giving a total of 14% (n 1⁄4 56) in the combined contemplation/preparation stage. Very few were in the action stage (2%, n 1⁄4 7), while over a quarter were in the maintenance stage (27%, n 1⁄4 105), giving a total of 28% (n 1⁄4 112) in the combined action/maintenance stage. A number of respondents (n 1⁄4 20) were unable to be classified.
There were statistically significant differences between stages of change groups for a number of food items (Table 2). Those in action/maintenance ate more serves of fruit and vegetables each day and ate nuts, seeds, whole-meal bread and cooked cereals more often than those in the other stages. It should be noted that vegetable consumption was low even for those in action/maintenance, with the mean number of serves being only 2.871.8/day for this group, although fruit consumption was adequate (2.671.5 serves). The action/ maintenance group ate white bread and red and white meat the least often, with those in precontemplation eating these foods most frequently. Dairy products were eaten most frequently by those in precontemplation, with those in contemplation/preparation eating them least often. There were no statistically significant differences between stages for legumes, fruit juice, breakfast cereals, fish/seafood and eggs.
There were no statistically significant sex, household income or education differences between stages of change groups. However, there were age and vegetarian status differences (data not tabulated). Age differences were not linear and therefore difficult to interpret, with the main difference being that those in the contemplation/prepara- tion were younger than those in the other two groups (mean of 47713 years for contemplation/preparation compared to 53715 years for the remaining two groups, Po0.05). The strongest differences were with regard to vegetarian status
Table 2 Means and s.d.’s for frequency of consumption of food items for stages of change groups (precontemplation, contemplation/preparation, action/maintenance), together with P-values from analysis of variance for comparisons between groups
(Po0.001), with those in the action/maintenance group being more likely to be semivegetarian or vegetarian than those in the other two groups. Over a fifth in this group considered themselves to be semivegetarian (22, 18 and 5% for action/maintenance, contemplation/preparation and precontemplation, respectively). There were only three vegetarian respondents, all of whom were classified in the action/maintenance group.
Four components with eigenvalues greater than unity were derived from principal components analysis of the benefit items, accounting for 60% of the variance. The factors are listed in Table 3 and were provisionally labelled: Well-being benefits, Weight and health benefits, Ethical benefits, and Convenience and financial benefits. Measures of internal consistency (Cronbach’s a) for items with a loading of 35 or over indicated that all of the factors had high internal consistency.
Five components were derived from principal components analysis of the barrier items, which accounted for 59% of the correlation matrix (Table 4). These were provisionally labelled: Personal barriers, Family and convenience barriers, Health barriers, ‘Junk’ food, shopping, eating out and financial barriers, and Information barriers. All five factors had high internal consistency.
Comparisons between mean benefit factor scores and stages of change showed statistically significant differences for all four factors (Table 5). Those in action/maintenance scored highest and those in precontemplation the lowest on all factors except Ethical benefits, for which the contempla- tion/preparation group scored highest. However, post hocpairwise comparisons found that the mean score for Ethical benefits was not significantly different between contempla- tion/preparation and action/maintenance.
Food items
Fruit (serves/day)
Vegetables (serves/day)
Nuts (times/day)
Seeds (times/day)
Legumes (times/day)
Fruit juice (times/day)
White bread (times/day)
Wholemeal/mixed grain bread (times/day)
Breakfast cereals (times/day)
Cooked cereals (e.g. pasta, rice, noodles) (times/day) Red meat (times/day)
White meat (times/day)
Fish/seafood (times/day)
Eggs (times/day)
Dairy (e.g. milk, cheese) (times/day)
Precontemplation
1.83a (1.01) 2.44a (1.24) 0.27a (0.33) 0.21a (0.36) 0.34a (0.34) 0.47a (0.47) 0.61a (0.58) 0.64a (0.58) 0.62a (0.48) 0.34a (0.31) 0.49a (0.33) 0.38a (0.24) 0.25a (0.23) 0.24a (0.26) 1.04a (0.64)
Contemplation/preparation mean (s.d.)
1.94a (1.05) 2.39a,b (1.19) 0.30a (0.34) 0.21a (0.43) 0.28a (0.23) 0.54a (0.51) 0.44b (0.48) 0.60a (0.55) 0.67a (0.46) 0.40a,b (0.27) 0.37b (0.28) 0.34a,b (0.20) 0.25a (0.21) 0.20a (0.19) 0.74b (0.47)
Action/maintenance
2.63b (1.52) 2.83b (1.84) 0.44b (0.44) 0.43b (0.50) 0.36a (0.32) 0.54a (0.50) 0.36b (0.53) 0.85b (0.61) 0.70a (0.42) 0.44b (0.34) 0.32b (0.22) 0.31b (0.17) 0.27a (0.21) 0.22a (0.18) 0.87b (0.54)
P-value 17.49 (2.389) ***
3.14 (2.388) *
8.30 (2.390) *** 12.29 (2.377) *** 1.13 (2.381) NS 0.91 (2.387) NS 7.74 (2.389) ***
5.35 (2.391) ** 1.14 (2.388) NS 3.94 (2.389) *
13.15 (2.390) *** 3.82 (2.389) * 0.45 (2.389) NS 0.96 (2.389) NS 7.46 (2.391) ***
F (df)
The smallest number of respondents for any individual item among groups was: precontemplation n1⁄4218; contemplation/preparation n1⁄451; and action/ maintenance n 1⁄4 109.
a,bFor each food item, means with different superscripts are statistically significantly different from each other (Po0.05).
***Po0.001, **Po0.01, *Po0.05, NS 1⁄4 not significant.
European Journal of Clinical Nutrition
Consumers’ readiness to eat a plant-based diet
EJ Lea et al
Table 4 Results of principal components analysis of the barriers towards
347
Table 3 Results of principal components analysis of the benefits of eating a plant-based diet
eating a plant-based diet
Factor and items
Factor 1: personal barriers
Eigenvalue: 10.20 Cronbach’s a: 0.89 Percent of variance: 39.2%
I don’t want to change my eating habit or routine It would not be filling enough
I don’t want people to think I’m strange or a hippy I think humans are meant to eat lots of meat
I wouldn’t get enough energy or strength
It would not be tasty enough
I would need to eat such a large quantity of plant foods I don’t want to eat strange or unusual foods
There is not enough choice when I eat out
I don’t know what to eat instead of lots of meat
It is inconvenient
Factor 2: family and convenience barriers
Eigenvalue: 1.70 Cronbach’s a: 0.86 Percent of variance: 6.5%
My family/partner won’t eat a plant-based diet
It takes too long to prepare plant-based meals Someone else decides on most of the food I eat
I don’t want to eat strange or unusual foods
The plant foods I would need are not available where I shop or in the canteen or at my home
It is inconvenient
I don’t know how to prepare plant-based meals I don’t have enough willpower
Factor 3: health barriers
Eigenvalue: 1.38 Cronbach’s a: 0.86 Percent of Variance: 5.3%
There is not enough iron in them
There is not enough protein in them
I would be worried about my health (other than iron, protein)
I would get indigestion, bloating, gas or flatulence
I wouldn’t get enough energy or strength
I would need to eat such a large quantity of plant foods
Factor 4: ‘Junk’ food, shopping, eating out and financial barriers
Eigenvalue: 1.09 Cronbach’s a: 0.83 Percent of variance: 4.2%
Factor and items
Factor 1: well-being benefits
Eigenvalue: 8.97 Cronbach’s a: 0.91 Percent of variance: 39.0%
Be more content with myself Have a better quality of life Stay healthy
Be fit
Have a tasty diet
Have plenty of energy
Eat a more ‘natural’ diet
Improve my digestion
Lower my chances of getting food poisoning Eat a greater variety of foods
Have lots of vitamins and minerals
Factor 2: weight and health benefits
Eigenvalue: 2.25 Cronbach’s a: 0.88 Percent of variance: 9.8%
Decrease my saturated fat intake Control my weight
Prevent disease in general
Eat more fibre
Have lots of vitamins and minerals Improve my digestion
Eat a more ‘natural’ diet
Stay healthy
Have plenty of energy
Factor 3: ethical benefits
Eigenvalue: 1.51 Cronbach’s a: 0.83 Percent of Variance: 6.6%
Decrease hunger in the Third World Help animal welfare/rights
Increase efficiency of food production Help the environment
Lower my chances of getting food poisoning Appear more ‘trendy’ to my friends
Factor 4: convenience and financial benefits
Eigenvalue: 1.04 Cronbach’s a: 0.76 Percent of variance: 4.5%
Save time
Have fewer food storage problems Save money
Eat a greater variety of foods
Have a tasty diet
Factor loadings
78 77 76 74 63 61 51 49 40 38 35
77 70 67 63 60 52 43 40 36
82 81 80 72 46 38
79 67 62 44 41
Factor loadings
Comparisons between the means of the barrier factor scores and stages of change found statistically significant differences present for all of the factors (Table 5). Those in precontemplation scored highest for Family and conveni- ence barriers and Health barriers, with those in action/ maintenance the lowest. They also scored the highest for
I would (or do) miss eating lots of junk food 72 I would have to go food shopping too often 71 Plant-based meals or snacks are not available when I 63 eat out
It would be too expensive 61 I don’t have enough willpower 39 There is not enough choice when I eat out 38 The plant foods I would need are not available where I 37 shop or in the canteen or at my home
Factor 5: information barriers
Eigenvalue: 1.08 Cronbach’s a: 0.81 Percent of variance: 4.2%
I need more information about plant-based diets 75 I don’t know how to prepare plant-based meals 60 I don’t know what to eat instead of lots of meat 57 I would need to eat such a large quantity of plant foods 39
It would not be filling enough
36
67 67 66 64 62 59 52 46 37 36 36
66 63 60 56 54
46 43 41
87 85 70
44 40 39
European Journal of Clinical Nutrition
348
Consumers’ readiness to eat a plant-based diet
EJLeaetal
Table 5 Means and s.d.’s for benefits and barriers of consumption of a plant-based diet factor scores for stages of change groups (precontemplation, contemplation/preparation, action/maintenance), together with P-values from analysis of variance for comparisons between groups
Precontemplation
0.40a (0.94) 0.20a (1.04) 0.15a (0.91) 0.15a (0.90)
0.25a (0.95) 0.16a (1.08) 0.15a (0.97) 0.05a (0.99)
0.09a (1.01)
Contemplation/preparation mean (s.d.)
0.40b (0.95) 0.17b (0.77) 0.17b (1.26)
0.08a (1.20) 0.56b (0.83)
0.09a (0.77) 0.02a,b (1.05) 0.33a (0.97)
0.52b (0.85)
Action/maintenance
0.61b (0.76)
0.41b (0.83) 0.07a,b (0.99) 0.34b (1.04)
0.41b (0.79) 0.47b (0.75) 0.28b (1.04) 0.20b (0.82)
0.38c (0.83)
F (df)
P-value
Benefit factors
Well-being benefits
Weight and health benefits
Ethical benefits
Convenience and financial benefits
Barrier factors
Personal barriers
Family and convenience barriers Health barriers
‘Junk’ food, shopping, eating out and
financial barriers Information barriers
46.44 (2.337) *** 13.77 (2.337) *** 2.96 (2.337) *
7.95 (2.337) ***
26.42 (2.333) *** 14.47 (2.333) *** 5.85 (2.333) **
5.40 (2.333) ** 16.18 (2.333) ***
The smallest number of respondents for any individual item among groups was: precontemplation n1⁄4191; contemplation/preparation n1⁄449; and action/ maintenance n 1⁄4 95.
a–cFor each food item, means with different superscripts are statistically significantly different from each other (Po0.05).
***Po0.001, **Po0.01, *Po0.05, NS 1⁄4 not significant.
Personal barriers, although in this case contemplation/ preparation rather than action/maintenance scored the low- est. The contemplation/preparation group scored highest for ‘Junk’ food, shopping, eating out and financial barriers and Information barriers, with action/maintenance the lowest.
The post hoc pairwise comparisons did not find three distinct stages of change (Tables 2 and 5). Where statistically significant differences were present, they tended to be between the first two stages (i.e. precontemplation, con- templation/preparation) and the final stage (action/main- tenance), between the first stage and the final two stages, or between the first and final stage. The only factor or item where a statistically significant difference was found between each stage was Information barriers.
Discussion
This is the first study that has been conducted on plant- based diets and stages of change, and thus may be considered exploratory. This study suggests that a large proportion of the population is not yet ready to consume a plant-based diet, with over half of the participants classified as being in the precontemplation stage. Over a quarter were in the maintenance stage, with very few being in action. The remainder were in contemplation or preparation. Previous research on stages of change for related eating behaviours (healthy eating, low-fat diets, and fruit, vegetable and grain consumption) has generally found fewer people to be in precontemplation and more to be in maintenance than was found here (Povey et al., 1999; Oˆ unpuu et al., 2000; Van Duyn et al., 2001; Ma et al., 2002; Greene et al., 2003). This may partly be due to the focus on the total diet, rather than specific aspects of the diet, such as vegetables or low fat. Eating a plant-based diet is presumably viewed as being more difficult to achieve, or, alternatively, as less desirable to
achieve. It may also be partly due to the novelty of the concept ‘plant-based diet’. Over half of the respondents (54%) had not heard of the term prior to participation in the survey, although there was no significant difference between prior awareness and stages of change.
There were strong differences across stages of change groups with regard to benefit and barrier factors. Those in action/maintenance perceived there to be well-being, weight, health, ethical, convenience and financial benefits of consuming a plant-based diet, whereas those in precon- templation did not recognise these benefits. For those who have not been exposed to the benefits of eating a plant-based diet, an awareness raising strategy could help to increase recognition and credibility. Ethical benefits were strongest among those in contemplation/preparation and action/ maintenance. This could be due to people with these kinds of altruistic values being attracted to plant foods because of their ethical connotations. However, it is possible that providing information on the food security, environmental and animal welfare benefits associated with a plant-based diet (Lewis, 1994; Pimentel and Pimentel, 2003) may help to progress people to the action stage. Those in contemplation/ preparation are likely to be more susceptible to such information than those who are not considering eating a plant-based diet, as they were found to lack information on plant-based diets. This group would also be likely to benefit from information on availability and preparation of heal- thier alternatives to foods such as confectionary and fast food, availability of suitable plant-based meals or snacks from food outlets and tips on how to decrease the number of shopping trips (or alternatively how to increase enjoyment of food shopping, such as by attending markets). Such messages could be targeted at younger people, as those in contemplation/preparation were younger than those in the other groups. Those in precontemplation have an even
European Journal of Clinical Nutrition
broader range of barriers to overcome before they would be likely to consume a plant-based diet, including health- and family-related barriers and an unwillingness to alter their current diet and reduce their meat consumption. These are the areas that should be focused on by those in the public health nutrition arena and others who wish to progress those in precontemplation with regard to plant-based diets to later stages. For example, education is required about how it is possible to obtain iron and protein from a plant-based diet, and tips on how to make a gradual, easy transition to a plant- based diet. Education and communication should be oriented to the whole family and should distinguish partner opposition as a potential practical constraint. Precontem- plators would also need to be provided with ample reasons to make this dietary change – that is, the benefits of change. Greater targeted awareness raising among opinion leaders is one method by which plant foods and plant-based diets could be promoted. Broader change is also required, such as the cooperation of food processors in the production and promotion of healthy and tasty plant-based foods that are attractive to the entire family.
The absence of sex differences between stages of change groups is noteworthy, as previous research has found that women tend to be more health conscious and more likely to be a low meat consumer than are men (Rappoport et al., 1993; Australian Bureau of Statistics, 1997; Fagerli and Wandel, 1999; Kearney et al., 2000; Lea and Worsley, 2004). In addition, a study on stages of change for fruit and vegetables found that those in maintenance were more likely to be women (Van Duyn et al., 1998). Women in the current study did eat significantly less red meat and more fruit and vegetables than did men (data not reported here). However, there were no significant sex differences with regard to other foods such as white meat, legumes, nuts, seeds and whole- meal bread, although women ate eggs significantly more often than did men (data not reported here). Perhaps when the whole diet is considered, rather than food items such as red meat, fruits and vegetables, there is a lack of sex differences with regard to consumption of a plant-based diet. The novelty of the term ‘plant-based diet’ and the possible lack of awareness about the benefits of consumption of a whole range of plant foods may play a role in this finding. Further research is required to corroborate this result.
It is plausible for there to be a difference in vegetarian status between those at the various stages of change to a plant-based diet. Owing to health being a common motiva- tion for choosing a vegetarian or semivegetarian diet (Beardsworth and Keil, 1991; Rozin et al., 1997; Lea and Worsley, 2003a,b) and to the reduced emphasis on meat, there is likely to be an increased emphasis on plant foods. Indeed, previous research has found that vegetarians do consume higher quantities of plant foods than do non- vegetarians (Perry et al., 2002; Haddad and Tanzman, 2003).
Previous research on self-determined stages of change for fruit and vegetable consumption has found that those in the
Consumers’ readiness to eat a plant-based diet
EJ Lea et al
higher stages tend to eat more fruits, vegetables and grains than those in lower stages (Van Duyn et al., 1998, 2001; Greene et al., 2003). In the current study, there were strong differences between stages of change categories and fre- quency of consumption of a variety of foods of plant and animal origin, despite the subjectivity and complexity of the definition of plant-based diet provided. Those in the highest stages ate more fruit, vegetables, nuts, seeds, whole-meal bread and cooked cereals than those in the other stages, which provide some evidence of validity to the use of self- determined measures of stage of change. However, vegetable and legume consumption was low for all respondents, including those in action or maintenance. The recom- mended daily intake of vegetables in Australia is five serves or more a day (National Health and Medical Research Council, 2003), so even those in action/maintenance were eating two serves too few. Other surveys have also shown vegetable consumption in Australia to be low (Australian Bureau of Statistics, 1997; Victorian Government Depart- ment of Human Services, 2004). One reason for this is that the public is often unaware of the quantity of vegetables that is recommended to be eaten (Lechner et al., 1997; Stables et al., 2002). The legume and vegetable food groups may therefore need to be the key focus of attempts to increase plant food consumption.
A limitation of the study was the modest response rate (51%). However, a response rate of 50% is considered adequate for reporting (Babbie, 1989), and other surveys conducted in Australia and elsewhere have had a lower response rate (Cox et al., 1998; Timperio et al., 2000). It would be useful to conduct a larger study to confirm and expand the present findings, particularly given the small size of some stage of change groups. A larger study could help to decide on the usefulness of the stages of change model for plant-based diets and other complex dietary behaviours, as the finding that the post hoc comparisons do not support the notion of discrete stages may be due to the need to combine some stages. Nonetheless, the study did find differences between stages in plant and animal food consumption, as well as perceived barriers and benefits of plant-based diets and sociodemographic characteristics, which does support the applicability of the stages of change model for plant- based diets. In addition, although we feel that the results of this study may be most appropriately used to devise a variety of public health messages to increase consumption of a broad range of plant foods, rather than messages that explicitly utilise the term ‘plant-based diet’, it would be interesting if future research were to compare the effective- ness of messages that incorporate this term with those that target the separate components of plant-based diets (e.g. ‘eat more vegetables’).
In conclusion, the study found that a large proportion of the population is not yet ready to consume a plant-based diet. The findings on the associations between stages of change, benefits and barriers of plant-based diets and demographic characteristic may be used to help encourage
349
European Journal of Clinical Nutrition
350
Consumers’ readiness to eat a plant-based diet
EJLeaetal
a higher consumption of plant foods and to help progress people from earlier to later stages of change with regard to a plant-based diet. They can be utilised to help provide appropriate nutrition education and advertising, targeted at specific stages of change. In particular, awareness raising of the benefits of plant-based diets and the need for change is necessary for those in precontemplation, while those in contemplation and preparation need practical information, such as on the availability and preparation of healthier foods compared to high-energy, low-nutrient foods. Environmen- tal supports, such as greater availability of plant-based meals in food outlets and government policies that support production, are likely to be useful for those at all stages of change. Future research could examine the willingness of opinion leaders and policy makers to encourage the public to increase their consumption of plant foods and plant-based diets.
Acknowledgements
The project was supported by the Australian Research Council (DP0209041). EJL is supported by an ARC Post- doctoral Fellowship. DC is supported by a National Health and Medical Research Council/National Heart Foundation Career Development Award.
References
Agudo A, Slimani N, Ocke ́ MC, Naska A, Miller AB, Kroke A et al. (2002). Consumption of vegetables, fruit and other plant foods in the European Prospective Investigation into Cancer and Nutrition (EPIC) cohorts from 10 European countries. Public Health Nutr 5, 1179–1196.
American Dietetic Association (2003). Position of the American Dietetic Association and Dietitians of Canada: vegetarian diets. J Am Diet Assoc 103, 748–765.
Ammerman AS, Lindquist CH, Lohr KN, Hersey J (2002). The efficacy of behavioral interventions to modify dietary fat and fruit and vegetable intake: a review of the evidence. Prev Med 35, 25–41.
Australian Bureau of Statistics (1997). National Nutrition Survey Selected Highlights Australia 1995. Australian Government Publish- ing Service: Canberra.
Australian Bureau of Statistics (2002). 2001 Census Basic Community Profile and Snapshot: Victoria. Australian Bureau of Statistics: Canberra.
Babbie E (1989). The Practice of Social Research. Wadsworth Publishing Company: Belmont, CA.
Bazzano LA, He J, Ogden LG, Loria CM, Vupputuri S, Myers L et al. (2002). Fruit and vegetable intake and risk of cardiovascular disease in US adults: the first National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey Epidemiologic Follow-up Study. Am J Clin Nutr 76, 93–99.
Beard L, Wyllie A, Caswell S (1989). Towards Understanding Eating Habits in New Zealand: A Qualitative Investigation. Department of Community Health, University of Auckland: Auckland.
Beardsworth A, Keil T (1991). Health-related beliefs and dietary practices among vegetarians and vegans: a qualitative study. Health Educ J 50, 38–42.
Campbell MK, Havas S, Jackson B, Damron D, McClelland J, Anliker J et al. (2001). 5 A Day research with African-American churches and
the Special Supplemental Nutrition Program for Women, Infants, and Children. In: Stables G, Heimendinger J (ed). 5 A Day for Better Health Program (Monograph). National Institutes of Health: Bethes- da, MD, pp 151–167.
Cox DN, Anderson AS, Lean MEJ, Mela DJ (1998). UK consumer attitudes, beliefs and barriers to increasing fruit and vegetable consumption. Public Health Nutr 1 (Suppl 2), 61–68.
de Graaf C, Van der Gaag M, Kafatos A, Lennernas M, Kearney JM (1997). Stages of dietary change among nationally-representative samples of adults in the European Union. Eur J Clin Nutr 51, S47–S56.
Dillman DA (2000). Mail and Internet Surveys: The Tailored Design Method. John Wiley: New York.
Fagerli RA, Wandel M (1999). Gender differences in opinions and practices with regard to a ‘healthy diet’. Appetite 32, 171–190. Finckenor M, Byrd-Bredbenner C (2000). Nutrition intervention
group program based on preaction-stage-oriented change pro- cesses of the Transtheoretical Model promotes long-term reduc- tion in dietary fat intake. J Am Diet Assoc 100, 335–342.
Fraser GE, Welch A, Luben R, Bingham SA, Day NE (2000). The effect of age, sex, and education on food consumption of a middle-aged English cohort – EPIC in East Anglia. Prev Med 30, 26–34.
Glanz K, Hoelscher D (2004). Increasing fruit and vegetable intake by changing environments, policy and pricing: restaurant-based research, strategies, and recommendations. Prev Med 39 (Suppl 2), S88–S93.
Glanz K, Yaroch AL (2004). Strategies for increasing fruit and vegetable intake in grocery stores and communities: policy, pricing, and environmental change. Prev Med 39 (Suppl 2), S75–S80.
Greene G, Horacek T, White A, Ma J (2003). Use of a diet interview method to define stages of change in young adults for fruit, vegetable, and grain intake. Top Clin Nutr 18, 32–41.
Haddad EH, Tanzman JS (2003). What do vegetarians in the United States eat? Am J Clin Nutr 78, S626–S632.
Hair JF, Anderson RE, Tatham RL, Black WC (1995). Multivariate Data Analysis. Prentice-Hall: Eaglewood Cliffs, NJ.
Horacek T, White A, Betts NM, Hoerr S, Georgiou C, Nitzke S et al. (2002). Self-efficacy, perceived benefits, and weight satisfaction discriminate among stages of change for fruit and vegetable intakes for young men and women. J Am Diet Assoc 102, 1466–1470.
Hunt CJ, Nichols RN, Pryer JA (2000). Who complied with national fruit and vegetable population goals? Eur J Public Health 10, 178–184.
Kearney JM, McElhone S (1999). Perceived barriers in trying to eat healthier – results of a pan-EU consumer attitudinal survey. Br J Nutr 81 (Suppl 2), S133–S137.
Kearney M, Gibney MJ, Martinez JA, de Almeida MDV, Friebe D, Zunft HJF et al. (1997). Perceived need to alter eating habits among representative samples of adults from all member states of the European Union. Eur J Clin Nutr 51 (Suppl 2), S30–S35.
Kearney M, Kearney JM, Dunne A, Gibney MJ (2000). Sociodemo- graphic determinants of perceived influences on food choice in a nationally representative sample of Irish adults. Public Health Nutr 3, 219–226.
Key TJ, Appleby PN, Davey GK, Allen NE, Spencer EA, Travis RC (2003). Mortality in British vegetarians: review and preliminary results from EPIC – Oxford. Am J Clin Nutr 78, S533–S538.
Lang R, Thane CW, Bolton-Smith C, Jebb SA (2003). Consumption of whole-grain foods by British adults: findings from further analysis of two national dietary surveys. Public Health Nutr 6, 479–484.
Lappalainen R, Saba A, Holm L, Mykkanen H, Gibney MJ (1997). Difficulties in trying to eat healthier: descriptive analysis of perceived barriers for healthy eating. Eur J Clin Nutr 51, S36–S40.
Lea E, Crawford C, Worsley A (2005a). Public views of the benefits and barriers to the consumption of a plant-based diet. Eur J Clin Nutr (accepted subject to revision).
European Journal of Clinical Nutrition
Lea E, Worsley A (2003a). Benefits and barriers to the consumption of a vegetarian diet in Australia. Public Health Nutr 6, 505–511.
Lea E, Worsley A (2003b). The factors associated with the belief that vegetarian diets provide health benefits. Asia Pac J Clin Nutr 12, 296–303.
Lea E, Worsley A (2004). What proportion of South Australian non- vegetarians hold similar beliefs to vegetarians? Nutr Diet 61, 11–21. Lea E, Worsley A, Crawford D (2005b). Australian adult consumers’ beliefs about plant foods: a qualitative study. Health Educ Behav (in
press).
Lechner L, Brug J, De Vries H (1997).
Misconceptions of fruit and
vegetable consumption: differences between objective and sub-
jective estimation of intake.
J Nutr Educ 29, 313–320.
Lewis S (1994).
An opinion on the global impact of meat consump-
tion.
Am J Clin Nutr 59, S1099–S1102.
Ling AMC, Horwath C (2000).
Defining and measuring stages of
change for dietary behaviors: readiness to meet fruit, vegetables and grains guidelines among Chinese Singaporeans. J Am Diet Assoc 100, 898–904.
Lloyd HM, Paisley CM, Mela DJ (1995). Barriers to the adoption of reduced-fat diets in a UK population. J Am Diet Assoc 95, 316–322. Ma J, Betts NM, Horacek T, Georgiou C, White A, Nitzke S (2002). The importance of decisional balance and self-efficacy in relation to stages of change for fruit and vegetable intakes by young adults.
Am J Health Prom 16, 157–166.
Marks GC, Webb K, Rutishauser I, Riley M (2001).
Monitoring Food
Habits in the Australian Population Using Short Questions. Common-
wealth Department of Health and Aged Care: Canberra. McIntosh WA, Kubena KS, Jiang H, Usery CP, Karnei K (1996). An application of the Health Belief Model to reductions in fat and
cholesterol intake.
J Wellness Perspect 12, 98–107.
Montonen J, Knekt P, Ja ̈rvinen R, Aromaa A, Reunanen A (2003).
Whole-grain and fiber intake and the incidence of type 2 diabetes.
Am J Clin Nutr 77, 622–629.
National Health and Medical Research Council (2003).
Dietary
Guidelines for Australian Adults. National Health and Medical
Research Council: Canberra.
Nestle M, Wing R, Birch L, DiSogra L, Drewnowski A, Middleton S
et al. (1998). Behavioral and social influences on food choice. Nutr
Rev 56, S50–S74.
Oˆunpuu S, Woolcott DM, Greene GW (2000).
Defining stage of
change for lower-fat eating.
J Am Diet Assoc 100, 674–679.
Perry CL, McGuire MT, Neumark-Sztainer D, Story M (2002).
Adolescent vegetarians: how well do their dietary patterns meet the healthy people 2010 objectives?
Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 156,
431–437.
Pimentel D, Pimentel M (2003).
Sustainability of meat-based and
plant-based diets and the environment. Am J Clin Nutr 78 (Suppl 3),
S660–S663.
Potter JD (2000).
Your mother was right: eat your vegetables.
Asia Pac
J Clin Nutr 9 (Suppl 1), S10–S12.
Povey R, Conner M, Sparks P, James R, Shepherd R (1999).
A critical
examination of the application of the Transtheoretical Model’s stages of change to dietary behaviours. Health Educ Res 14, 641–651.
Prochaska JO, DiClemente CC, Norcross JC (1992). In search of how people change: applications to addictive behaviors. Am Psychol 47, 1102–1114.
Consumers’ readiness to eat a plant-based diet
EJ Lea et al
Prochaska JO, Velicer WF, Rossi JS, Goldstein MG, Marcus BH, Rakowski W et al. (1994). Stages of change and decisional balance for 12 problem behaviors. Health Psychol 13, 39–46.
Rappoport L, Peters G, Downey R, McCann T, Huff-Corzine L (1993). Gender and age differences in food cognition. Appetite 20, 33–52.
Rosenstock IM (1974). Historical origins of the Health Belief Model. Health Educ Monogr 2, 328–335.
Rozin P, Markwith M, Stoess C (1997). Moralization and becoming a vegetarian: the transformation of preferences into values and the recruitment of disgust. Psychol Sci 8, 67–73.
Rutishauser I, Webb K, Abraham B, Allsopp R (2001). Evaluation of Short Dietary Questions from the 1995 National Nutrition Survey. Commonwealth Department of Health and Aged Care: Canberra.
Schwartz SH (1992). Universals in the content and structure of values: theoretical advances and empirical tests in 20 countries. Adv Exp Soc Psychol 25, 1–65.
Stables GJ, Subar AF, Patterson BH, Dodd K, Heimendinger J, Van Duyn MAS et al. (2002). Changes in vegetable and fruit consump- tion and awareness among US adults: results of the 1991 and 1997 5 A Day for Better Health Program surveys. J Am Diet Assoc 102, 809–817.
Stubenitsky K, Mela DJ (2000). UK consumer perceptions of starchy foods. Br J Nutr 83, 277–285.
Timperio A, Cameron-Smith D, Burns C, Crawford D (2000). The public’s response to the obesity epidemic in Australia: weight concerns and weight control practices of men and women. Public Health Nutr 3, 417–424.
Van Duyn MAS, Heimendinger J, Russek-Cohen E, DiClemente CC, Sims LS, Subar AF et al. (1998). Use of the Transtheoretical Model of Change to successfully predict fruit and vegetable consump- tion. J Nutr Educ 30, 371–380.
Van Duyn MAS, Kristal AR, Dodd K, Campbell MK, Subar AF, Stables GJ et al. (2001). Association of awareness, intrapersonal and interpersonal factors, and stage of dietary change with fruit and vegetable consumption: a national survey. Am J Health Promot 16, 69–78.
Victorian Government Department of Human Services (2004). Victorian Population Health Survey 2003: Selected Findings. Victorian Government Department of Human Services: Melbourne.
Wardle J, Steptoe A (2003). Socioeconomic differences in attitudes and beliefs about healthy lifestyles. J Epidemiol Community Health 57, 440–443.
Wolinsky FD (1980). The Sociology of Health: Principles, Professions and Issues. Little/Brown: Boston, MA.
World Cancer Research Fund and American Institute for Cancer Research (1997). Food, Nutrition and the Prevention of Cancer: A Global Perspective. American Institute for Cancer Research: Washington, DC.
World Health Organization (2003). Fruit and Vegetable Promotion Initiative. A Meeting Report. WHO: Geneva.
Worsley A, Blasche R, Ball K, Crawford D (2003). Income differences in food consumption in the 1995 Australian National Nutrition Survey. Eur J Clin Nutr 57, 1198–1211.
Zunft HJF, Friebe D, Seppelt B, de Graaf C, Margetts B, Schmitt A et al. (1997). Perceived benefits of healthy eating among a nationally- representative sample of adults in the European Union. Eur J Clin Nutr 51 (Suppl 2), S41–S46.
Logging in, please wait...
0 archived comments